WikiJournal of Science/Multiple object tracking

From Wikiversity
Jump to navigation Jump to search

WikiJournal of Science
Open access • Publication charge free • Public peer review • Wikipedia-integrated

WikiJournal of Science is an open-access, free-to-publish, Wikipedia-integrated academic journal for science, mathematics, engineering and technology topics. WJS WikiJSci Wiki.J.Sci. WikiJSci WikiSci WikiScience Wikiscience Wikijournal of Science Wikiversity Journal of Science WikiJournal Science Wikipedia Science Wikipedia science journal STEM Science Mathematics Engineering Technology Free to publish Open access Open-access Non-profit online journal Public peer review

<meta name='citation_doi' value='10.15347/WJS/2023.003'>

Article information

Submitting author: Alex O. Holcombe[a][i] 
Additional contributors: Wikipedia community

See author information ▼
  1. School of Psychology, The University of Sydney
  1. alex.holcombe@sydney.edu.au

Abstract

In psychology and neuroscience, multiple object tracking (MOT) refers to the ability of humans and other animals to simultaneously monitor multiple objects as they move. It is also the term for a laboratory technique used to study this ability.

In an MOT study, a number of identical moving objects are presented on a display. Some of the objects are designated as targets while the rest serve as distractors. Study participants try to monitor the changing positions of the targets as they and the distractors move about. At the end of the trial, participants typically are asked to indicate the final positions of the targets.

The results of MOT experiments have revealed dramatic limitations on humans' ability to simultaneously monitor multiple moving objects. For example, awareness of features such as color and shape is disrupted by the objects' movement.


Introduction

History

In the 1970s, the researcher Zenon Pylyshyn postulated the existence of a "primitive visual process" in the human brain capable of “indexing and tracking features or feature-clusters”. Using this process, cognitive processes can continuously refer to, or "track", objects despite movement of the objects causing them to stimulate different visual neurons over time.[1] Data collected with Pylyshyn's multiple object tracking (MOT) protocol and published in 1988 provided the first formal demonstration that the mind can keep track of the changing positions of multiple moving objects.[1]

As a specific theory of this ability, Pylyshyn proposed FINST (Fingers of Instantiation) theory, which claims that tracking is mediated by a fixed set of discrete pointers. While FINST theory has been very influential, many studies have found evidence that seems inconsistent with the theory.[2]

Procedure

Sequence of events in a typical MOT task.
Teeeea, CC-BY-SA 4.0

A typical MOT study involves the presentation of between eight and twelve objects. The participant is told to monitor the positions of a subset of the objects, which are referred to as targets. Often the targets are indicated by being presented initially in a distinct color. The targets then become identical in appearance to the other, distractor objects. The targets and distractors move about the screen for several seconds in an unpredictable fashion. The participant is then asked to indicate which of the objects are the targets. The accuracy of the participant's judgments indicates whether the participant mentally updated the positions of the targets as they moved.

To ensure that the task requires participants to mentally update the targets' positions, displays are typically designed such that object paths cause the targets to swap positions with distractors, at least occasionally. With that constraint, MOT task variations have been designed to probe specific aspects of how the mind tracks moving objects. For example, to compare performance in the left to performance in the right visual fields, studies confine some or all the moving objects to one of the visual fields.[3] To avoid any contribution from spatial interference among mental object representations, some studies maintain a minimum distance between objects.[4] Other studies have combined MOT with a concurrent task to investigate whether the two tasks draw on the same mental resource, and have changed target features such as color to assess whether study participants update their representations of those features.

Capacity limits

MOT study results indicate that the number of targets that people can track is very limited. This reflects a bottleneck in the brain's processing architecture. While at the early, sensory stages of visual processing, dozens of objects may be fully processed, later processes such as those associated with cognition have much more limited capacity to process visual objects.[5]

The specific number of visual objects that people can accurately track varies widely with display parameters, contrary to a common belief that people can track no more than four or five objects. Even for a fixed set of display parameters, rather than there being a clear limit, performance falls gradually with the number of targets.[6] Such findings undermine Pylyshyn's FINST theory that tracking is mediated by a fixed set of discrete pointers.[7]

The above limitations appear to stem from processes specific to the two cerebral hemispheres. The independence of the limits in the two hemifields is demonstrated by findings that when one is tracking the maximum number that can be tracked in the left hemifield (which is processed by the right cerebral hemisphere), one can add targets to the right hemifield (which is processed by the left cerebral hemisphere) at little to no cost to performance.[8][9] For features other than position, capacity seems to be more limited - see the "Updating of features other than position" section below.

While the tracking capacity limit is largely set separately by the two cerebral hemispheres, a more unified and cognitive resource also can contribute to tracking. For example, if there is only one target, one can bring one's full cognitive abilities to bear, such as in predicting future positions, to facilitate tracking. When more targets are present, these resources may still play a role.[10]

Spatiotemporal limits

If the objects of a display are not sufficiently widely spaced, the objects are difficult to identify and select with attention due to spatial crowding,[11] which can prevent tracking.[4][12] High object speeds have a similar effect - faster objects are harder to track, and humans are completely unable to track objects that move sufficiently fast. This "speed limit", however, is much slower than the maximum object speed at which humans can judge the object's movement direction.[9][13] This dissociation between motion perception and object tracking is thought to reflect that direction judgments can be based on low-level and local motion detector responses that don't register the positions of objects.

As an object's speed is increased, temporal crowding can result and prevent tracking well before the tracking speed limit is reached.[13][14] Temporal crowding refers to an impairment caused by distractors visiting a target's former location within a short interval. The phenomenon was revealed in a study with a display where distractors were evenly-spaced along a circular trajectory that was also shared by a target. Participants could not track three targets if the locations traversed were visited by objects more than three times per second, and this was true even if the objects were moving at a relatively slow speed. This temporal crowding limit on tracking becomes more severe as the number of targets increases.[14][15]

As the spatial, temporal, and speed limits are approached, tracking performance decreases gradually[12][14] and in typical MOT displays, it is unclear which of these limits, or what combination of them, determine the maximum number of targets that can be tracked.[16] For the spatial limit, one study found little to no effect beyond the Bouma's law crowding zone.[17] Many MOT studies do not enforce sufficient spacing between objects to avoid spatial crowding, making spatial crowding likely to be one factor in overall performance.

Role of prediction and trajectory information

Brains continuously predict some aspects of the future.[18][19] Human object tracking performance is higher when object trajectories are predictable, suggesting that prediction also occurs in this domain. However, the benefit seems to disappear when there are more than one or two targets,[20][21] suggesting that any prediction happening is more limited in processing capacity than other aspects of object tracking. In those studies, however, predictability of objects' future positions appears to be confounded with the objects being distinguishable from each other on the basis of maintaining particular and different motion directions. In such experiments, the difference in targets' and distractors' motion directions or accelerations may be the facilitator of tracking rather than prediction of future positions.[22] Indeed, distinctiveness of motion directions alone facilitates tracking,[22] thus any role played by the prediction of future positions remains unclear. However, performance detecting a change in a target's trajectory is much worse with each increase in target number, which suggests little ability to utilize trajectory information.[23]

Role of grouping and coordinate frames

The human brain represents the positions of objects with multiple reference frames or coordinate systems. Early stages of the visual system represent the locations of objects relative to the direction the eyes are pointing (retinotopic coordinates). Some later stages of human visual processing can represent object locations relative to each other or to the scene.

Regarding representation of relative locations, the relative positions of objects can be represented with an imaginary polygon, with each target a different vertex of that polygon. In studies of MOT, Steve Yantis drew participants' attention to the polygon formed by the targets and found that benefited performance,[24] as did setting the targets' trajcetories to avoid much disruption of the constantly-morphing polygon. This suggests that shape tracking contributes to accurate performance, at least in some participants.[25] One study measured an electrical brain response (ERP) to a probe that was flashed while the objects were moving. The earliest-detectable part of the neural response to the probe was significantly greater if the probe lay on the polygon defined by the targets rather than inside or outside the polygon.[26] This suggests that at least some of the participants continuously tracked the polygon defined by the targets.

Displays with more complicated statistical relationships among moving targets have been devised to show that regularities in hierarchical relationships are extracted and utilized in multiple object tracking, including nesting of groups of objects within moving reference frames.[27]

Updating of features other than position

The classic MOT task requires updating of targets' positions but not their other features. People appear to be less able to update the other features of targets, and have difficulty even in maintaining their knowledge of such features as the associated objects move. In one study, Pylyshyn assigned distinct identities to four identical targets, either by giving them names or by giving them easily-identifiable starting positions: the four corners of the screen. In addition to the usual task at the end of the trial of identifying which objects were the targets, participants also were asked about the identity of the targets - which one each was. Contrary to what Pylyshyn expected from his FINST theory, accuracy at identifying which target was which was very low, even when accuracy reporting the targets' positions was high.[28]

To assess maintenance of knowledge of object identities, one series of experiments used cartoon animals as targets and distractors that all moved about the screen. By the end of each trial, the animals came to rest behind cartoons of cacti, so that their identities were no longer visible. Participants were asked where a particular target (e.g., the cartoon rabbit) had gone - that is, which occluder it was hiding behind. In this multiple identity tracking (MIT) task, performance was much worse than in the standard MOT task of reporting target locations irrespective of which target a location belonged to.[29]

The deficit in updating the locations of featural and identity information may reflect a more general deficit in updating the locations of objects in visual short-term memory. In a study using a shell game in which the shells hid brightly-colored balls of wool, pairs of shells were swapped at a slow rate of once a second, but accuracy judging which shell contained a particular color fell to 80% accuracy when there were four swaps in a simple three-shell display, compared to 95% accuracy for four swaps with a two-shell display.[30]

The concept of an "object file" is that of a record in the brain that stores the features of a visual object, with the location record updated as the object moves.[31] In the original studies that were motivated by this idea, one feature an object disappears and the object moves to a new location. The feature is then presented in the new location, and people respond faster to that feature than to features that were not previously presented as part of the object. This finding of priming indicates that an object file was created and updated by the brain. One might expect this to tap into the same processing as that assessed by the MIT task. The relationship between the two is unclear, however, as there is evidence that attentional tracking occurs can occur along a different trajectory than that which is the basis of updating the memory of an object's features.[32]

In the studies mentioned so far, the objects involved did not change any of their features besides their positions, so the task was to maintain knowledge of (unchanging) features while updating their positions. Change blindness studies show that in many circumstances, people do poorly at noticing that features have changed. A famous demonstration involves placing a blank screen between the presentation of two versions of a screen to mask the flicker that would otherwise be associated with a change. Change blindness also occurs when the flicker evoked by the change is masked by the objects' motion.[33][34] That, however, may only mean that nothing is comparing the features present before and after the change; it does not necessarily mean that object representations are not updated, so other studies are needed.

A related issue is whether tracking can occur on the basis not only of smooth changes in the position of an object, but also on the basis of smooth changes in an object's other features. In a tracking experiment in which two objects were always spatially superposed, the objects maintained their separate identities based on smooth continuity of their colors, orientations, and spatial frequencies. The participants could only track one such object,[35] suggesting no ability to capitalize on spatiotemporal feature continuity for features other than position, although this has not yet been tested for cases in which the targets do not overlap (overlap may trigger figure-ground interference).

Difficulty tracking unusual objects and object parts

Many objects have clearly-visible parts. A dumbbell, for example, has a central bar part and has the weights at the bar's ends. Even when such parts are conspicuous, people can have difficulty tracking an individual part of multiple objects. When individual ends of multiple dumbbell-shaped drawings are designated as targets, tracking performance is poor.[36][37] Performance was even worse when participants attempted to track one end of multiple moving lines, where the lines were uniform without distinct parts. Evidently, the mental processes that underlie tracking of multiple objects operate on a particular type of object representation that differs from what we can consciously recognize. Possibly the representation used for tracking is shared by that used when searching for a particular colored shape that is hidden among many other shapes; visual search is hindered by connecting targets to distractors.[38][39]

For some types of "objects" that are not segmented as such by early visual processing, not even a single instance can be tracked. Stuart Anstis has shown that people are unable to track the intersection of two lines sliding over each other, except possibly at very slow speeds.[40]

Some things change shape as they move, such as liquids and slinkys. For slinky-like objects that moved by extending their leading edges to a point and then retracting their trailing edges, Kristy vanMarle and Brian Scholl found that tracking performance was poor.[41] The underlying reason for this is unclear, but reporting the location of even a lone object is impaired by growth or contraction of the object, which may contribute to the tracking failure.[36]

Interference with concurrent performance of other tasks

Overlap among the processes underlying mental abilities can be revealed by what types of concurrent tasks interfere with each other. Attempting to track multiple visual objects typically interferes with other tasks,[42] even for tasks with stimuli in other modalities.[43][44] Unfortunately, it can be difficult to determine whether this reflects processing somewhat specific to our ability to track or instead reflects the processing necessary to initiate and sustain a wide variety of tasks.

One exception to the usual finding of interference with other tasks is that an auditory pitch discrimination task was found to not interfere with visual multiple object tracking.[45] With a task designed as an auditory analog of tracking rather than just requiring discrimination of a few pitches, however, Daryl Fougnie et al. found that the task interfered approximately as much with visual object tracking as did a visual feature-tracking task. This suggests that auditory and visual tracking are limited by a common processing resource.[46]

Neural basis

Neuroimaging studies find that activation of areas of the parietal cortex increases with the number of objects tracked, which is consistent with the suggestion that the parietal cortex plays a role in humans' limited tracking capacity.[47][48][49] Activation of other brain areas also seems to increase with target load, but the particular areas may be less consistent across studies than the parietal cortex finding. The size of participants' pupils also increases with the number of objects tracked. The pupil size increase, which also is caused by mental effort in other tasks, may reflect norepinephrine release from the locus coeruleus.[49][50]

Objects presented to the left visual hemifield are processed initially by the right cerebral hemisphere, while stimuli presented to the right visual hemifield are processed initially by the left cerebral hemisphere. The independent capacity limits in the two hemifields are very similar, although there may be a small right-hemifield advantage.[51] A right hemifield advantage would be consistent with a contribution by both parietal cortices to tracking that hemifield, which was suggested because both parietal cortices are thought to contribute to other attentional functions in the right hemifield.[52]

The neural basis of MOT has also been studied using electroencephalography (EEG). One such study found a robust correlation between tracking performance and the effect of number of targets on the N2pc event-related potential and also on contralateral delay activity.[53] Multiple brain areas contribute to these signals, so such studies have not yet allowed researchers to determine exactly which brain areas mediate tracking.

Human variation and development

The scores of a person are usually similar to each other if they are tested multiple times in the same MOT experiment (high task reliability).[54][55][56][57] This suggests that the variation in the number of objects people seem able to track (for one version of the task, capacities ranged between one and six targets)[58][59] reflects real variation in ability. One caveat is that studies have failed to assess how much of this could be due to variation in individuals' motivation. At least one study, however, tested only top military recruits, a sample that was likely to be well-motivated.[59]

Rather than samples from a broad population, most research has been conducted on undergraduates at universities in Western countries. Some studies, however, have tested other populations. Comparing children of different ages, two studies in North America found a marked increase with age in the number of objects the children could track, from 6 or 7 years old to adulthood.[60][61] For people with autism spectrum disorders, two studies found that such participants had poorer MOT performance than typically-developing participants, which was attributed to a deficit in attentional selection.[62][63]

Adults with Williams Syndrome have profound deficits on certain spatial assembly tasks, such as copying a four-block checkerboard pattern.[64] For multiple object tracking, their performance is similar to typically-developing four- or five-year-old children.[65][62][63] In contrast, their ability to remember the locations of MOT targets if they don't move is more comparable to typically-developing 6-year-olds, which has led to the suggestion that maintaining attentional selection is a particular problem in Williams Syndrome.[66]

Among older typically-developing adults, MOT performance falls steeply with age.[15][67][68] Age-related increases in spatial crowding[69] and temporal crowding[15] likely contribute to this.

Several papers report that video game players perform substantially better in MOT tasks than those who don't play video games.[61][70] However, it has been suggested that this could be an artifact of research practices such as selective publication of results.[71]

Covariation of object tracking ability with other abilities

While some have used MOT in an attempt to ensure study participants sustain their attention over a long interval, a study with a large number of participants found little correlation with a continuous performance task specifically designed to measure lapses in attention.[72] MOT may, then, be forgiving of lapses in attention, which is consistent with findings that for typical displays, participants can perform well in MOT even if they are occasionally briefly interrupted, with their tracking processes able to pick up where they left off.[42][73]

One approach to investigating which tasks share underlying processing is to test participants on several different tasks to determine which tasks have the highest correlations across individuals. The results of studies that have done this with MOT have not been entirely consistent with each other, so which tasks yield the highest correlation with MOT performance is not yet clear. However, multiple studies find that visual working memory is one of the most highly-correlated tasks.[54][56] The correlation is consistent with broader findings that working memory tasks are among the best predictors of performance in a wide range of tasks.[74] Possibly this reflects shared underlying mechanisms such as maintaining goal-relevant information in memory (possibly including which objects are the targets) and disengaging from outdated or irrelevant information.[75]

Use in ability testing and training

Some professional sports teams use laboratory-style MOT tests for ability assessment and for training.[76] Associates of the company that makes the "NeuroTracker" MOT product claim that NeuroTracker is a "cognitive enhancer" that improves a variety of abilities relevant to performance on the sports field, but the evidence in the studies purporting to show this is weak.[77] Another reason for skepticism of such claims is the poor track record of other commercial "brain training" products advertised for their cognitive-enhancing effects.[76][78]

While it is unlikely that training on laboratory-style MOT tasks yields broad mental benefits, when more rigorous studies are done, it is possible that firm evidence may support the use of tasks related to MOT for screening or training purposes for specific purposes. Regarding screening, however, one study found that laboratory MOT performance did not predict driving test performance as well as the Montreal Cognitive Assessment, a trail-making task, or a useful field-of-view task.[79] A multiple object avoidance (MOA) task, involving steering a ball with a computer mouse to prevent it from colliding with other moving balls on a computer screen, was found to correlate better with driving performance than MOT.[80] In another study, strong positive correlations with MOA performance were found with driving simulator performance and years of driving experience.[81] This may be because MOA includes control of movement, which is necessary for driving, but is not required for MOT.[82]

Theories and models

Published computational models fit some aspects of tracking results, with most focusing on the pattern of performance decline with increasing number of targets, and some modeling the dissociation between position and non-position features. No published theory purports to explain all four of the following: the difficulty with tracking parts of objects, the role of temporal interference, the dissociation between position and non-positional features, and the pattern of performance decline with increasing number of targets.

Serial versus parallel processing

The independence of tracking in the left and right hemifields suggests that position updating in each hemifield occurs independently of and in parallel with position updating in the other hemifield (see the "Capacity limits" section above). Within a hemifield, it is not yet completely clear whether tracking of multiple objects happens in parallel or instead the target positions are updated one-by-one, but most recent theorists agree with Pylyshyn's original FINST theory that positions are updated in parallel.[83][84][85][86] A finding that gives some support to the alternative of serial switching is the marked increase in temporal interference as the number of targets tracked increases. In particular, the amount of increase in time needed between when a target leaves a location and a distractor takes its place is approximately predicted by the theory that attention must visit each moving target one-by-one to update its location.[14]

Some who theorize that position updating occurs simultaneously for multiple targets draw a contrast with features other than position, stating that they are updated by a process that must serially switch among the targets.[83][84][85][86] A model of Lovett, Bridewell, & Bello published in 2019, for example, includes a parallel process to track changes in position and connect to visual pointers that are shared with visual short-term memory and other visual attention tasks. A serial selection process is also included, which operates on only one object at a time and enables access to a target's motion history and other features.[87]

Slots versus resources

Central to Pylyshyn's FINST theory is that a small set of discrete pointers mediate multiple object tracking. Subsequent researchers have suggested that rather than discrete pointers, a mental resource that is more continuous is divided among the targets.[88][89] This dispute is similar to the "slots versus resources" debate in the study of working memory. A continuous resource naturally explains the smooth decline in performance with number of targets, although there is no agreement about what precisely about tracking becomes worse when less resource is provided. Possibilities include spatial resolution, temporal resolution, the maximum speed of the tracker, or all three (see the "Spatiotemporal limits" section above).

Conclusion

The apparent dissociation between knowledge of object locations and knowledge of their other features may be related to the role of attentional selection in feature binding. According to feature integration theory, serial attention is required to bind together individual non-position features with their locations. Because multiple object tracking appears to involve attending to multiple features simultaneously, it follows from feature integration theory that feature updating cannot be done for multiple objects. What was not necessarily expected from feature integration theory is that features or identifies of multiple moving objects cannot even be maintained. Treisman's contribution to influential foundational work on object files may have led many researchers to expect that once objects are bound, they would be maintained. Perhaps bound object files should be conceived of as being "hard to move".

Serial attention may also be needed to create the full representations of objects that we are familiar with from introspection during focused attention. Without time for serial attention to visit individual objects, tracking may have to rely on proto-objects,[39] which could explain humans' difficulty in tracking parts of objects.[36][37]

While tracking capacity is independent in the left and right visual hemifields, the neural basis of this has been hardly investigated, so we know very little about this hemisphere-specific visual attention process. For example, we do not know whether the interference documented with other tasks is mediated by hemisphere-specific processes. The temporal crowding phenomenon described in the "Spatiotemporal limits" section is also relevant to interference with other tasks. Researchers rarely analyze how often target locations must be sampled to avoid confusion with distractors. For some of the MOT displays typically used, one might be able to attend to another task for hundreds of milliseconds and still be able to recover the targets based on proximity to their last location. Future studies should use displays where the temporal crowding level is assessed and controlled, as in those of Holcombe & Chen.[14] This may result in higher correlations with tasks that measure whether attention is continuously sustained than in the study by Fortenbaugh et al.[72]

Additional information

Competing interests

The author has no competing interests, other than that the article cites some of the author's research, including a scholarly book (for which the author does not earn royalties).

Ethics statement

No original research on humans was conducted for this article.

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 Pylyshyn, Z. W.; Storm, R. W. (1988). "Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism". Spatial Vision 3 (3): 179–197. doi:10.1163/156856888X00122. 
  2. Scholl, Brian J. (2008). "What Have We Learned about Attention from Multiple-Object Tracking (and Vice Versa)?". In Dedrick, Don; Trick, Lana. Computation, cognition, and Pylyshyn. MIT Press. pp. 49–78. doi:10.7551/mitpress/8135.003.0005. ISBN 9780262255196. 
  3. Edwards, Grace; Berestova, Anna; Battelli, Lorella (2021-09-29). "Behavioral gain following isolation of attention". Scientific Reports 11 (1): 19329. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-98670-w. ISSN 2045-2322. PMID 34588526. PMC PMC8481494. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41598-021-98670-w. 
  4. 4.0 4.1 Holcombe, A. O.; Chen, W.- Y.; Howe, P. D. L. (2014-08-01). "Object tracking: Absence of long-range spatial interference supports resource theories". Journal of Vision 14 (6): 1–1. doi:10.1167/14.6.1. ISSN 1534-7362. http://jov.arvojournals.org/Article.aspx?doi=10.1167/14.6.1. 
  5. Holcombe, Alex O. (2023). Attending to moving objects. Cambridge University Press. Section 2. doi:10.1017/9781009003414. ISBN 9781009003414. 
  6. Holcombe 2023, Section 3.
  7. Alvarez, George A.; Franconeri, Steven L. (2007-10-30). "How many objects can you track?: Evidence for a resource-limited attentive tracking mechanism". Journal of Vision 7 (13): 14. doi:10.1167/7.13.14. ISSN 1534-7362. http://jov.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?doi=10.1167/7.13.14. 
  8. Alvarez, George A.; Cavanagh, Patrick (2005-08). "Independent Resources for Attentional Tracking in the Left and Right Visual Hemifields". Psychological Science 16 (8): 637–643. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01587.x. ISSN 0956-7976. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01587.x. 
  9. 9.0 9.1 Holcombe, Alex O.; Chen, Wei-Ying (2012-05). "Exhausting attentional tracking resources with a single fast-moving object". Cognition 123 (2): 218–228. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.003. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010027711002459. 
  10. Holcombe 2023, Section 6.
  11. "Visual crowding". Wikipedia. 2021-11-30. https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Visual_crowding&oldid=1058017413. 
  12. 12.0 12.1 Intriligator, James; Cavanagh, Patrick (2001-11). "The Spatial Resolution of Visual Attention". Cognitive Psychology 43 (3): 171–216. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0755. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010028501907558. 
  13. 13.0 13.1 Verstraten, Frans A.J; Cavanagh, Patrick; Labianca, Angela T (2000-12). "Limits of attentive tracking reveal temporal properties of attention". Vision Research 40 (26): 3651–3664. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00213-3. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0042698900002133. 
  14. 14.0 14.1 14.2 14.3 14.4 Holcombe, A. O.; Chen, W.-Y. (2013-01-09). "Splitting attention reduces temporal resolution from 7 Hz for tracking one object to". Journal of Vision 13 (1): 12–12. doi:10.1167/13.1.12. ISSN 1534-7362. http://jov.arvojournals.org/Article.aspx?doi=10.1167/13.1.12. 
  15. 15.0 15.1 15.2 Roudaia, Eugenie; Faubert, Jocelyn (2017-09-01). "Different effects of aging and gender on the temporal resolution in attentional tracking". Journal of Vision 17 (11): 1. doi:10.1167/17.11.1. ISSN 1534-7362. http://jov.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?doi=10.1167/17.11.1. 
  16. Holcombe 2023, Section 4.
  17. Holcombe, A. O.; Chen, W.- Y.; Howe, P. D. L. (2014-08-01). "Object tracking: Absence of long-range spatial interference supports resource theories". Journal of Vision 14 (6): 1–1. doi:10.1167/14.6.1. ISSN 1534-7362. http://jov.arvojournals.org/Article.aspx?doi=10.1167/14.6.1. 
  18. Clark, Andy (2016). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Oxford. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190217013.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-021701-3. OCLC 904011681. https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/904011681. 
  19. Hohwy, Jakob (2013). The predictive mind (First edition ed.). Oxford. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199682737.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-150519-5. OCLC 868923880. https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/868923880. 
  20. Howe, P. D. L.; Holcombe, A. O. (2012-12-10). "Motion information is sometimes used as an aid to the visual tracking of objects". Journal of Vision 12 (13): 10–10. doi:10.1167/12.13.10. ISSN 1534-7362. http://jov.arvojournals.org/Article.aspx?doi=10.1167/12.13.10. 
  21. Luu, Tina; Howe, Piers D. L. (2015-08). "Extrapolation occurs in multiple object tracking when eye movements are controlled". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 77 (6): 1919–1929. doi:10.3758/s13414-015-0891-8. ISSN 1943-3921. http://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13414-015-0891-8. 
  22. 22.0 22.1 Wang, Yang; Vul, Edward (2021-03-26). "The role of kinematic properties in multiple object tracking". Journal of Vision 21 (3): 22. doi:10.1167/jov.21.3.22. ISSN 1534-7362. https://jov.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?articleid=2772432. 
  23. Tripathy, Srimant P.; Barrett, Brendan T. (2004-12-09). "Severe loss of positional information when detecting deviations in multiple trajectories". Journal of Vision 4 (12): 4. doi:10.1167/4.12.4. ISSN 1534-7362. https://doi.org/10.1167/4.12.4. 
  24. Yantis, Steven (1992-07). "Multielement visual tracking: Attention and perceptual organization". Cognitive Psychology 24 (3): 295–340. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(92)90010-Y. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/001002859290010Y. 
  25. Merkel, Christian; Stoppel, Christian M.; Hillyard, Steven A.; Heinze, Hans-Jochen; Hopf, Jens-Max; Schoenfeld, Mircea Ariel (2014-01-01). "Spatio-temporal Patterns of Brain Activity Distinguish Strategies of Multiple-object Tracking". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 (1): 28–40. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00455. ISSN 0898-929X. https://direct.mit.edu/jocn/article/26/1/28/28040/Spatio-temporal-Patterns-of-Brain-Activity. 
  26. Merkel, Christian; Hopf, Jens-Max; Schoenfeld, Mircea Ariel (2017-02). "Spatio-temporal dynamics of attentional selection stages during multiple object tracking". NeuroImage 146: 484–491. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.046. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053811916306024. 
  27. Bill, Johannes; Pailian, Hrag; Gershman, Samuel J.; Drugowitsch, Jan (2020-09-29). "Hierarchical structure is employed by humans during visual motion perception". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 117 (39): 24581–24589. doi:10.1073/pnas.2008961117. ISSN 0027-8424. PMID 32938799. PMC PMC7533882. https://pnas.org/doi/full/10.1073/pnas.2008961117. 
  28. Pylyshyn, Zenon (2004-10). "Some puzzling findings in multiple object tracking: I. Tracking without keeping track of object identities". Visual Cognition 11 (7): 801–822. doi:10.1080/13506280344000518. ISSN 1350-6285. http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/13506280344000518. 
  29. Horowitz, Todd S.; Klieger, Sarah B.; Fencsik, David E.; Yang, Kevin K.; Alvarez, George A.; Wolfe, Jeremy M. (2007-02). "Tracking unique objects". Perception & Psychophysics 69 (2): 172–184. doi:10.3758/BF03193740. ISSN 0031-5117. http://link.springer.com/10.3758/BF03193740. 
  30. Pailian, Hrag; Carey, Susan E.; Halberda, Justin; Pepperberg, Irene M. (2020-12). "Age and Species Comparisons of Visual Mental Manipulation Ability as Evidence for its Development and Evolution". Scientific Reports 10 (1): 7689. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-64666-1. ISSN 2045-2322. PMID 32376944. PMC PMC7203154. http://www.nature.com/articles/s41598-020-64666-1. 
  31. Kahneman, Daniel; Treisman, Anne; Gibbs, Brian J (1992-04). "The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information". Cognitive Psychology 24 (2): 175–219. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(92)90007-O. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/001002859290007O. 
  32. Mitroff, Stephen R.; Scholl, Brian J.; Wynn, Karen (2005-05). "The relationship between object files and conscious perception". Cognition 96 (1): 67–92. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.03.008. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010027704001490. 
  33. Saiki, J.; Holcombe, A. O. (2012-03-06). "Blindness to a simultaneous change of all elements in a scene, unless there is a change in summary statistics". Journal of Vision 12 (3): 2–2. doi:10.1167/12.3.2. ISSN 1534-7362. http://jov.arvojournals.org/Article.aspx?doi=10.1167/12.3.2. 
  34. Suchow, Jordan W.; Alvarez, George A. (2011-01). "Motion Silences Awareness of Visual Change". Current Biology 21 (2): 140–143. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.019. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0960982210016507. 
  35. Blaser, Erik; Pylyshyn, Zenon W.; Holcombe, Alex O. (2000-11). "Tracking an object through feature space". Nature 408 (6809): 196–199. doi:10.1038/35041567. ISSN 0028-0836. http://www.nature.com/articles/35041567. 
  36. 36.0 36.1 36.2 Howe, Piers D.; Incledon, Natalie C.; Little, Daniel R. (2012-07-30). de Fockert, Jan. ed. "Can Attention Be Confined to Just Part of a Moving Object? Revisiting Target-Distractor Merging in Multiple Object Tracking". PLoS ONE 7 (7): e41491. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041491. ISSN 1932-6203. PMID 22859990. PMC PMC3408494. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041491. 
  37. 37.0 37.1 Scholl, Brian J; Pylyshyn, Zenon W; Feldman, Jacob (2001-06). "What is a visual object? Evidence from target merging in multiple object tracking". Cognition 80 (1-2): 159–177. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00157-8. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010027700001578. 
  38. Holcombe 2023, Section 7.4.
  39. 39.0 39.1 Wolfe, Jeremy M.; Bennett, Sara C. (1997-01). "Preattentive Object Files: Shapeless Bundles of Basic Features". Vision Research 37 (1): 25–43. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00111-3. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0042698996001113. 
  40. Anstis, S. (1990). Imperceptible intersections: The chopstick illusion. In A. Blake and T. Troscianko (Eds.), AI and the Eye. London: Wiley and Sons Ltd., 105-117.
  41. vanMarle, Kristy; Scholl, Brian J. (2003-09). "Attentive Tracking of Objects Versus Substances". Psychological Science 14 (5): 498–504. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.03451. ISSN 0956-7976. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9280.03451. 
  42. 42.0 42.1 Alvarez, George A.; Horowitz, Todd S.; Arsenio, Helga C.; DiMase, Jennifer S.; Wolfe, Jeremy M. (2005). "Do Multielement Visual Tracking and Visual Search Draw Continuously on the Same Visual Attention Resources?". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 31 (4): 643–667. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.4.643. ISSN 1939-1277. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0096-1523.31.4.643. 
  43. Wahn, Basil; König, Peter (2015-07-29). "Audition and vision share spatial attentional resources, yet attentional load does not disrupt audiovisual integration". Frontiers in Psychology 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01084. ISSN 1664-1078. PMID 26284008. PMC PMC4518141. http://journal.frontiersin.org/Article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01084/abstract. 
  44. Wahn, Basil; Wahn, Basil; König, Peter (2015). "Vision and Haptics Share Spatial Attentional Resources and Visuotactile Integration Is Not Affected by High Attentional Load". Multisensory Research 28 (3-4): 371–392. doi:10.1163/22134808-00002482. ISSN 2213-4794. https://brill.com/view/journals/msr/28/3-4/article-p371_10.xml. 
  45. Arrighi, Roberto; Lunardi, Roy; Burr, David (2011). "Vision and Audition Do Not Share Attentional Resources in Sustained Tasks". Frontiers in Psychology 2. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00056. ISSN 1664-1078. PMID 21734893. PMC PMC3110771. http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00056/abstract. 
  46. Fougnie, Daryl; Cockhren, Jurnell; Marois, René (2018-08). "A common source of attention for auditory and visual tracking". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 80 (6): 1571–1583. doi:10.3758/s13414-018-1524-9. ISSN 1943-3921. PMID 29717471. PMC PMC6061001. http://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13414-018-1524-9. 
  47. Jovicich, Jorge; Peters, Robert J.; Koch, Christof; Braun, Jochen; Chang, Linda; Ernst, Thomas (2001-11-15). "Brain Areas Specific for Attentional Load in a Motion-Tracking Task". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13 (8): 1048–1058. doi:10.1162/089892901753294347. ISSN 0898-929X. https://direct.mit.edu/jocn/article/13/8/1048/3594/Brain-Areas-Specific-for-Attentional-Load-in-a. 
  48. Culham, Jody C; Cavanagh, Patrick; Kanwisher, Nancy G (2001-11). "Attention Response Functions". Neuron 32 (4): 737–745. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00499-8. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896627301004998. 
  49. 49.0 49.1 Alnaes, D.; Sneve, M. H.; Espeseth, T.; Endestad, T.; van de Pavert, S. H. P.; Laeng, B. (2014-04-01). "Pupil size signals mental effort deployed during multiple object tracking and predicts brain activity in the dorsal attention network and the locus coeruleus". Journal of Vision 14 (4): 1–1. doi:10.1167/14.4.1. ISSN 1534-7362. http://jov.arvojournals.org/Article.aspx?doi=10.1167/14.4.1. 
  50. Wahn, Basil; Ferris, Daniel P.; Hairston, W. David; König, Peter (2016-12-15). Price, Nicholas Seow Chiang. ed. "Pupil Sizes Scale with Attentional Load and Task Experience in a Multiple Object Tracking Task". PLOS ONE 11 (12): e0168087. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0168087. ISSN 1932-6203. PMID 27977762. PMC PMC5157994. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168087. 
  51. Holcombe 2023, Section 9.6.
  52. Mesulam, M.-Marsel (1999-07-29). Howseman, A.; Zeki, S.. eds. "Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 354 (1387): 1325–1346. doi:10.1098/rstb.1999.0482. ISSN 0962-8436. PMID 10466154. PMC PMC1692628. https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rstb.1999.0482. 
  53. Drew, T.; Vogel, E. K. (2008-04-16). "Neural Measures of Individual Differences in Selecting and Tracking Multiple Moving Objects". Journal of Neuroscience 28 (16): 4183–4191. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0556-08.2008. ISSN 0270-6474. PMID 18417697. PMC PMC2570324. https://www.jneurosci.org/lookup/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0556-08.2008. 
  54. 54.0 54.1 Huang, Liqiang; Mo, Lei; Li, Ying (2012-04). "Measuring the interrelations among multiple paradigms of visual attention: An individual differences approach.". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 38 (2): 414–428. doi:10.1037/a0026314. ISSN 1939-1277. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0026314. 
  55. Wilbiks, Jonathan M. P.; Beatteay, Annika (2020-10). "Individual differences in multiple object tracking, attentional cueing, and age account for variability in the capacity of audiovisual integration". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 82 (7): 3521–3543. doi:10.3758/s13414-020-02062-7. ISSN 1943-3921. https://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13414-020-02062-7. 
  56. 56.0 56.1 Treviño, Melissa; Zhu, Xiaoshu; Lu, Yi Yi; Scheuer, Luke S.; Passell, Eliza; Huang, Grace C.; Germine, Laura T.; Horowitz, Todd S. (2021-12). "How do we measure attention? Using factor analysis to establish construct validity of neuropsychological tests". Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications 6 (1): 51. doi:10.1186/s41235-021-00313-1. ISSN 2365-7464. PMID 34292418. PMC PMC8298746. https://cognitiveresearchjournal.springeropen.com/articles/10.1186/s41235-021-00313-1. 
  57. Eayrs, Joshua; Lavie, Nilli (2018-08). "Establishing individual differences in perceptual capacity.". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 44 (8): 1240–1257. doi:10.1037/xhp0000530. ISSN 1939-1277. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/xhp0000530. 
  58. Meyerhoff, Hauke S.; Papenmeier, Frank (2020-12). "Individual differences in visual attention: A short, reliable, open-source, and multilingual test of multiple object tracking in PsychoPy". Behavior Research Methods 52 (6): 2556–2566. doi:10.3758/s13428-020-01413-4. ISSN 1554-3528. https://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13428-020-01413-4. 
  59. 59.0 59.1 Oksama, Lauri; Hyönä, Jukka (2004-07). "Is multiple object tracking carried out automatically by an early vision mechanism independent of higher‐order cognition? An individual difference approach". Visual Cognition 11 (5): 631–671. doi:10.1080/13506280344000473. ISSN 1350-6285. http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13506280344000473. 
  60. Trick, Lana M.; Jaspers-Fayer, Fern; Sethi, Naina (2005-07-01). "Multiple-object tracking in children: The “Catch the Spies” task". Cognitive Development 20 (3): 373–387. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2005.05.009. ISSN 0885-2014. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201405000249. 
  61. 61.0 61.1 Dye, Matthew W. G.; Bavelier, Daphne (2010-02-22). "Differential development of visual attention skills in school-age children". Vision Research. Perceptual Learning Part II 50 (4): 452–459. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.10.010. ISSN 0042-6989. PMID 19836409. PMC PMC2824025. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S004269890900474X. 
  62. 62.0 62.1 Koldewyn, Kami; Weigelt, Sarah; Kanwisher, Nancy; Jiang, Yuhong (2013-06). "Multiple Object Tracking in Autism Spectrum Disorders". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43 (6): 1394–1405. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1694-6. ISSN 0162-3257. PMID 23104619. PMC PMC3581699. http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10803-012-1694-6. 
  63. 63.0 63.1 O'Hearn, Kirsten; Franconeri, Steven; Wright, Catherine; Minshew, Nancy; Luna, Beatriz (2013-04). "The development of individuation in autism.". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 39 (2): 494–509. doi:10.1037/a0029400. ISSN 1939-1277. PMID 22963232. PMC PMC3608798. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0029400. 
  64. Mervis, Carolyn B.; Robinson, Byron F.; Pani, John R. (1999-11). "Visuospatial Construction". The American Journal of Human Genetics 65 (5): 1222–1229. doi:10.1086/302633. PMID 10521286. PMC PMC1288273. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0002929707621272. 
  65. Ferrara, Katrina; Hoffman, James E.; O’Hearn, Kirsten; Landau, Barbara (2016-08-07). "Constraints on Multiple Object Tracking in Williams Syndrome: How Atypical Development Can Inform Theories of Visual Processing". Journal of Cognition and Development 17 (4): 620–641. doi:10.1080/15248372.2016.1195389. ISSN 1524-8372. https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/15248372.2016.1195389. 
  66. O’Hearn, Kirsten; Hoffman, James E.; Landau, Barbara (2010-05). "Developmental profiles for multiple object tracking and spatial memory: typically developing preschoolers and people with Williams syndrome: Multiple object tracking in preschool children and WS". Developmental Science 13 (3): 430–440. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00893.x. PMID 20443964. PMC PMC2927133. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00893.x. 
  67. Sekuler, Robert; McLaughlin, Chris; Yotsumoto, Yuko (2008-06). "Age-Related Changes in Attentional Tracking of Multiple Moving Objects". Perception 37 (6): 867–876. doi:10.1068/p5923. ISSN 0301-0066. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1068/p5923. 
  68. Kennedy, G. J.; Tripathy, S. P.; Barrett, B. T. (2009-02-01). "Early age-related decline in the effective number of trajectories tracked in adult human vision". Journal of Vision 9 (2): 21–21. doi:10.1167/9.2.21. ISSN 1534-7362. http://jov.arvojournals.org/Article.aspx?doi=10.1167/9.2.21. 
  69. Scialfa, C. T.; Cordazzo, S.; Bubric, K.; Lyon, J. (2013-07-01). "Aging and Visual Crowding". The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 68 (4): 522–528. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbs086. ISSN 1079-5014. https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/geronb/gbs086. 
  70. Green, C. S.; Bavelier, D. (2006-08-01). "Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: the case of action video game players". Cognition 101 (1): 217–245. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.10.004. ISSN 0010-0277. PMID 16359652. PMC PMC2896820. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027705001873. 
  71. Hilgard, Joseph; Sala, Giovanni; Boot, Walter R.; Simons, Daniel J. (2019-01-01). "Overestimation of Action-Game Training Effects: Publication Bias and Salami Slicing". Collabra: Psychology 5 (1). doi:10.1525/collabra.231. ISSN 2474-7394. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.231. 
  72. 72.0 72.1 Fortenbaugh, Francesca C.; DeGutis, Joseph; Germine, Laura; Wilmer, Jeremy B.; Grosso, Mallory; Russo, Kathryn; Esterman, Michael (2015-09). "Sustained Attention Across the Life Span in a Sample of 10,000: Dissociating Ability and Strategy". Psychological Science 26 (9): 1497–1510. doi:10.1177/0956797615594896. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 26253551. PMC PMC4567490. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0956797615594896. 
  73. Horowitz, Todd S.; Birnkrant, Randall S.; Fencsik, David E.; Tran, Linda; Wolfe, Jeremy M. (2006-06). "How do we track invisible objects?". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 13 (3): 516–523. doi:10.3758/BF03193879. ISSN 1069-9384. http://link.springer.com/10.3758/BF03193879. 
  74. Redick, Thomas S.; Engle, Randall W. (2006-07). "Working memory capacity and attention network test performance". Applied Cognitive Psychology 20 (5): 713–721. doi:10.1002/acp.1224. ISSN 0888-4080. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.1224. 
  75. Mashburn, Cody A.; Tsukahara, Jason S.; Engle, Randall W. (2020-11-05). Individual Differences in Attention Control: Implications for the Relationship Between Working Memory Capacity and Fluid Intelligence (in en). Oxford University Press. pp. 175–211. doi:10.1093/oso/9780198842286.003.0007. ISBN 978-0-19-884228-6. https://academic.oup.com/book/31963/chapter/267698194. 
  76. 76.0 76.1 Schonbrun, Zach (2017-01-04). "Keep Your Eye on the Balls to Become a Better Athlete". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 2022-10-06.
  77. Vater, Christian; Gray, Rob; Holcombe, Alex O. (2021-10). "A critical systematic review of the Neurotracker perceptual-cognitive training tool". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 28 (5): 1458–1483. doi:10.3758/s13423-021-01892-2. ISSN 1069-9384. PMID 33821464. PMC PMC8500884. https://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13423-021-01892-2. 
  78. Simons, Daniel J.; Boot, Walter R.; Charness, Neil; Gathercole, Susan E.; Chabris, Christopher F.; Hambrick, David Z.; Stine-Morrow, Elizabeth A. L. (2016-10). "Do “Brain-Training” Programs Work?". Psychological Science in the Public Interest 17 (3): 103–186. doi:10.1177/1529100616661983. ISSN 1529-1006. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1529100616661983. 
  79. Bowers, Alex R.; Anastasio, R. Julius; Sheldon, Sarah S.; O’Connor, Margaret G.; Hollis, Ann M.; Howe, Piers D.; Horowitz, Todd S. (2013-10). "Can we improve clinical prediction of at-risk older drivers?". Accident Analysis & Prevention 59: 537–547. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2013.06.037. PMID 23954688. PMC PMC3769510. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0001457513002650. 
  80. Mackenzie, Andrew K.; Harris, Julie M. (2017-02). "A link between attentional function, effective eye movements, and driving ability.". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 43 (2): 381–394. doi:10.1037/xhp0000297. ISSN 1939-1277. PMID 27893270. PMC PMC5279462. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/xhp0000297. 
  81. Mackenzie, Andrew K.; Vernon, Mike L.; Cox, Paul R.; Crundall, David; Daly, Rosie C.; Guest, Duncan; Muhl-Richardson, Alexander; Howard, Christina J. (2022-06). "The Multiple Object Avoidance (MOA) task measures attention for action: Evidence from driving and sport". Behavior Research Methods 54 (3): 1508–1529. doi:10.3758/s13428-021-01679-2. ISSN 1554-3528. https://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13428-021-01679-2. 
  82. Holcombe 2023, Section 12.
  83. 83.0 83.1 Oksama, Lauri; Hyönä, Jukka (2016-01). "Position tracking and identity tracking are separate systems: Evidence from eye movements". Cognition 146: 393–409. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.10.016. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010027715300949. 
  84. 84.0 84.1 Li, Jie; Oksama, Lauri; Hyönä, Jukka (2019-01). "Model of Multiple Identity Tracking (MOMIT) 2.0: Resolving the serial vs. parallel controversy in tracking". Cognition 182: 260–274. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.016. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010027718302749. 
  85. 85.0 85.1 Lovett, Andrew; Bridewell, Will; Bello, Paul (2019-12-23). "Selection enables enhancement: An integrated model of object tracking". Journal of Vision 19 (14): 23. doi:10.1167/19.14.23. ISSN 1534-7362. https://jov.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?articleid=2757905. 
  86. 86.0 86.1 Kazanovich, Yakov; Borisyuk, Roman (2006-06). "An Oscillatory Neural Model of Multiple Object Tracking". Neural Computation 18 (6): 1413–1440. doi:10.1162/neco.2006.18.6.1413. ISSN 0899-7667. https://direct.mit.edu/neco/article/18/6/1413-1440/7124. 
  87. Lovett, Andrew; Bridewell, Will; Bello, Paul (2019-12-23). "Selection enables enhancement: An integrated model of object tracking". Journal of Vision 19 (14): 23. doi:10.1167/19.14.23. ISSN 1534-7362. https://jov.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?articleid=2757905. 
  88. Alvarez, George A.; Franconeri, Steven L. (2007-10-30). "How many objects can you track?: Evidence for a resource-limited attentive tracking mechanism". Journal of Vision 7 (13): 14. doi:10.1167/7.13.14. ISSN 1534-7362. https://doi.org/10.1167/7.13.14. 
  89. Vul, E.; Frank, M.; Tenenbaum, J.; Alvarez, G. A. (2009). "Explaining human multiple object tracking as resource-constrained approximate inference in a dynamic probabilistic model". In Bengio, Y.; Schuurmans, D.; Lafferty, J. et al.. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. 22. pp. 1955–1963. ISBN 9781615679119. http://books.nips.cc/papers/files/nips22/NIPS2009_0980.pdf.