Motivation and emotion/Book/2023/Interoception and mental health

From Wikiversity
Jump to navigation Jump to search
Interoception and mental health:
What is the role of interoception in mental health?

Overview[edit | edit source]

Scenario

Imagine you are a university student who has been studying for the past two hours. You need to empty your bladder so you decide to go to the toilet because it might also be a good chance to not only stretch your legs, but to also give your brain a break from reading lengthy textbooks. Without realising, your body's interoceptive processes have brought these signals into conscious awareness as a way to maintain your physical and mental homeostasis. Once you come back from your toilet break, your brain can focus better, you will be able to sit more comfortably, and you will no longer have a full bladder.

Figure 1. University students sitting and studying. These students will eventually need to stand up to have a brain break, go to the toilet, and stretch their legs
Figure 2. The major biological systems used for interception, including: heart, kidneys, bladder, skin, hormones, lungs, stomach intestines, bone and immune cells.

How do you know if you’re feeling frightened? Do you feel your heart rate increase? Do your hands begin to tremble? Maybe, you begin to have trouble breathing. These bodily functions are our undercover, interoceptive senses that help to ensure the maintenance of our wellbeing.

Interoception refers to the process where the body communicates its internal state through bodily signals via the nervous system. These processes are a way to maintain homeostasis across all major biological systems (see Figure 2) and motivate us to act on these signals. When the body’s set-point of homeostasis is unstable, it helps people become aware of this through interoceptive processes such as breath quickening, increased heart rate, hunger cues and bladder fullness. If these processes are not working as they should, interoception make this known through things like nausea and pain (Weir, 2023).

When bodily processes are in communication with our brain, interoceptive processes are brought into conscious awareness – this is known as interoceptive awareness. Once in conscious awareness, the brain uses attention, learning, memory, emotion and cognition to ensure the body maintains its homeostatic state (Berntson and Khalsa 2021). For example, the brain knows the feeling of a full bladder means it needs to be emptied, so it will motivate us to go to the toilet. Because this phenomenon has effects on so many bodily processes, research surrounding this topic is vital. With the help of psychological and biological science, we can begin to explain and treat mental disorders and health issues in new ways.

The relationship between psychology and physiology has long been a point of research. Although interoception is commonly understood in the medical field, it is less understood in the psychology field. Fast growing research has begun to discover a relationship between bodily signals and mental health. The contents of this chapter will begin to explain how exactly these two interrelated, and which theory supports it (Khoury et al., 2018).

Focus Questions

• What theories help us explain how interoceptive awareness/deficits are related?

• How are neurological processes responsible for the interoceptive experience? • How does interoceptive dysregulation affect emotional regulation? • What is the relationship between interoceptive processes and mental health?

What theories help us explain how interoceptive awareness/deficits are related?[edit | edit source]

[Provide more detail]

James-Lange theory of emotion[edit | edit source]

This [which?] theory attempts to explain what causes emotion. James-Lange (1885) hypothesised that physical change in the body as a result of external stimuli, and the brain’s interpretation of this, leads to the experience of emotion (Cherry, 2022). For example, if you go to the movies and watch a film with a sad scene midway through, you may begin to cry. Your interpretation of this physiological response elicits the emotion of sadness.

Figure 3. shows the process of the James-Lange theory of emotion - event/stimulus, physical arousal, and interpretation of arousal which leads to an emotional experience.

Using this [which?] theory that emotions are a result of our interpretation of physical reactions to external stimuli, it would help to explain interoception’s role in mental health. If emotions are induced by changing states in the body, then people who are typically perceptive to interoceptive signals will experience ‘normal’ emotional reactions to external stimuli. However, individuals who cannot accurately perceive their interoceptive signals, might fail to experience emotions as they are supposed to (Prinz, 2006).

This theory has been criticised because of its heavy focus on responses that are a large part of the fight-or-flight response in which responses do not change for each emotion (e.g., heart rate, breathing). It is also criticised for its focus on physiological reactions coming before emotional experience – some people argue that emotional experience responds faster than physiological reactions (Reeve, 2018).

So, in the context of interoception and emotion, the question is whether the ability to perceive physiological/interoceptive processes causes emotion or if it is a result of emotional reactions form external stimuli. Modernised approaches to this theory have neglected the significance James-Lange placed on physical arousal causing emotion. It is now believed that emotions trigger physiological responses to adapt to external stimuli. So, according to the revised James-Lange theory of emotion, interoceptive processes may not cause emotion but they are certainly a necessary component of the experience of emotion (Reeve, 2018).

The James-Lange theory of emotion suggests that:

emotions resulting from external stimuli cause physiological reactions
if you watch a sad movie you will cry
emotions are a result of our interpretation of our physical reactions to external stimuli


Cannon-Bard theory of emotion[edit | edit source]

Figure 4. An example of the Cannon-Bard theory of emotion where physical and emotional reactions are experienced simultaneously when a dog barks.

The Cannon-Bard theory of emotion (1927) proposes that we experience emotions and physiological reactions that coincide, simultaneously. In this sense, this theory differs from other theories because both physical and emotional experience rely on one or the other to occur. According to this theory, the thalamus sends a signal to the amygdala and the autonomic nervous system (ANS) that causes a physical reaction in the body (e.g., trembling, sweating). Interoceptive signals are therefore experienced at the same time as an emotion. However, researchers have since criticised the theory for its little emphasis on other areas of the brain that are also responsible for emotion and the physical response (Cherry, 2020).

Schachter and Singer's Two-Factor Theory of Emotion (1962)[edit | edit source]

Schachter and Singer’s two-factor theory of emotion (1962) describes how emotion occurs under two components. First, the physical arousal that comes from a stimulus, and second, the cognitive label a person puts to the emotion resulting from the stimulus. Essentially, this theory describes that a stimulus causes physical arousal in which the brain uses the environment to associate with an emotion (Mcleod, 2023).

For example:

  1. Seeing a bear on a bush walk (stimulus)
  2. Feeling your heart race (physical arousal)
  3. Noticing that your increased heart rate is due to fear (cognitive label)
  4. You become aware that you are frightened (conscious experience of the emotion)

(Cherry, 2019)

According to this [which?] theory, the perception of physical arousal (interoception) during a significant event leads to the cognitive label of an emotion, and thus, the conscious experience of that emotion.

How are neurological processes responsible for the interoceptive experience?[edit | edit source]

Figure 5. The location of the insult cortex in the human brain

Interoceptive signals originate from sensory receptors in the body which relay through the spinal cord and brainstem and into the posterior insular cortex. There are many brain regions responsible for these signals, namely, the insula cortex and cingulate cortex.[factual?]

The posterior insula cortex is the primary area for interoceptive feelings in the body such as pain, temperature, itches, touch, hunger, thirst, and gustation (taste). The functions of the posterior insula are important as it functions in the subcortical brain and creates unconscious awareness of changes in bodily states (Reeve, 2018).The anterior insula on the other hand, represents these bodily changes through subjective conscious awareness via homeostatic and visceral sensations (e.g., hunger and satiety). It also processes positive and negative emotions towards oneself and others. Thus, those with an anterior insula deficit may have a lack of empathy for others as seen in some mental disorders (Reeve, 2018).

A review by Pasin Neto et al. (2021) also emphasises the importance of the insula cortex and cingulate cortex in interoception and emotion. According to the authors, both the insula and cingulate cortices are activated during emotion. In those with mental disorders, this area is likely to be dysfunctional. Evidence of these area’s activation is shown in imaging reports where subjects experience a variety of feelings including anger, fear, sadness, happiness etc. The anterior insular cortex in particular was activated during all these emotions. This is because it is significantly responsible the integration of physiology and emotion in conscious awareness.

Although the experience of interoception and emotion does occur due to the firing of neurons, modern approaches suggest they are no share a relationship with external stimuli also[factual?].

1 An example of interoception is:

falling over
cooking a meal when hungry
writing an essay

2 The insula cortex is solely responsible for unconscious awareness of homeostatic and visceral states.

True
False

How does interoceptive dysregulation affect emotional regulation?[edit | edit source]

Because interoception studies are relatively recent, it is not yet well known in the psychiatric field. However, neuroscience studies are finding more in-depth conclusions about interoceptive pathways. This helps to integrate recognising bodily sensations into therapeutic interventions to assist both interoceptive dysregulation and mental disorders.[factual?]

People usually learn labels for internal states through association in their childhood from their caregivers (e.g. being fed when your belly is rumbling). Although interoceptive experiences may fluctuate in adolescence and childhood, some people have reoccurring difficulties when labelling and understanding changing internal states that continues into adulthood. It is still debated whether interoceptive dysregulation causes mental health issues or vice-versa, however, it is likely a bidirectional relationship between the two (Brewer et al., 2021).

Alexithymia

Alexithymia is a neurodevelopmental condition that is characterised by having difficulty recognising, describing, and expressing emotions. This condition can affect emotion, physiological signals, and cognition (Legg, 2020). The lack of interoceptive accuracy/awareness is a large component of alexithymia. Using measures that test one’s ability to perceive physiological functions (e.g., heart rate and taste), a relationship has been found between increased alexithymia and decreased interoceptive accuracy. Although research surrounding this relationship is limited, there is certainly evidence for alexithymia to explain how decreased interoceptive accuracy is linked to emotional dysregulation and poor mental health (Brewer et al., 2021).

Interoceptive dysregulation or interoceptive atypicality is the heightened awareness of bodily sensations, or lack thereof, that attempt to maintain a homeostatic state – both physiologically and emotionally. It is often caused by a hypo-activation or hyper-activation of areas in the brain (often in the insular cortex). The inability to perceive and regulate certain bodily sensations (e.g., heart rate, hunger, reaction to positive stimuli) can exacerbate or stem from mental health issues because of its association with emotional suppression instead of emotional regulation (Brewer et al., 2021). For example, a person’s inability to feel satiated after eating can bring on, or perpetuate, restrictive eating habits as a result of deliberately suppressing interoceptive hunger cues, which leads to decreased interoceptive accuracy (Brewer et al., 2021; Weir, 2023). Interestingly, interoceptive accuracy has also been found to predict better emotional regulation and cognitive reappraisal[factual?]. However, this is not always a positive trait and can be seen as atypical in some cases and associations with social anxiety, accurate emotional memory (problematic in PTSD), and intense emotional experiences due to one’s heightened sense of internal bodily changes (Brewer et al., 2021).

The link between interoception and emotional regulation is an important topic to better understand ways to improve mental health using interoceptive techniques. Having the ability to regulate interoceptive processes is a core component of rehabilitation in many mental disorders such as anorexia nervosa (Brewer et al., 2021). Higher interoceptive accuracy is thought to improve emotional regulation after negative experiences through identification of useful strategies and cognitive reappraisal (Brewer et al., 2021).

Study - Interoception and mental health

A study conducted among young adult women by Brewer et al. (2021), tested interoceptive abilities and its relationship with emotional regulation. The study did so by using a heartbeat counting task and a self-report questionnaire to ask about subject’s perception of emotions, bodily sensations, and how they regulate emotional states in during their day-to-day life. The results of this study found most subjects to find the heartbeat counting task difficult, but it was especially difficult for subjects with decreased interoception. Participants categorised with high interoception reported being able to better regulate their emotions, were more self-focused, mindful, and motivated to improve their life through psychotherapy. These traits are associated with improved mental health, suggesting that people with higher interoceptive accuracy may be better equipped to improve their mental health.

What is the relationship between interoceptive processes and mental health?[edit | edit source]

Mental health and interoception have a newly researched relationship. The following table (table 1.) provides some of the conscious interoceptive signals that are linked with mental illness.

Table 1. conscious interoceptive signals that coincide with certain mental illnesses (Khalsa et al., 2018).

Mental Illness Interoceptive signals
Panic disorders heart palpitations, chest pain, nausea, dizziness, depersonalisation/derealisation
Depression increased or decreased appetite and sleep, slowed psychomotor functioning
Eating disorders hunger insensitivity, gastrointestinal pain
Substance abuse cravings, intoxication, withdrawal symptoms
PTSD increased heart rate and blood pressure, autonomic hyper vigilance
Generalised anxiety disorder muscle tension, headaches, fatigue, gastrointestinal pain, nausea

Major depressive disorder[edit | edit source]

Figure 6. A person using psychotherapy to help work through mental health issues such as MDD.

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a mental illness characterised by (but not limited to) chronic feelings of low positive affect (anhedonia), fatigue, lack of sleep, difficulty concentrating and making decisions, and sometimes thoughts of death or suicide (Torres, 2020).

Interoception maximises experiences of pleasure through informed decision-making as a way to maintain homeostasis. So, in those with MDD, this would imply that their anhedonia is related to decreased interoceptive processes that would typically motivate pleasure-seeking decisions, (Furman et al., 2013). People with MDD have difficulty remembering the feeling of previously enjoyed positive experiences which research suggests is a result of decreased interoceptive abilities (Furman et al., 2013), stemming from a hypo-activation of the insular cortex (Khoury et al., 2018). Without this ability to seek previously pleasurable activities, people with MDD only further sustain their lack of interoception and therefore negative affect.

Anorexia nervosa[edit | edit source]

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a serious eating disorder and mental illness. It is the third most common chronic illness among adolescent girls and women and is characterised by an intense fear of gaining weight, restriction of energy intake, and distorted body image (Australia, 2019).

People with AN often have difficulties distinguishing hunger and satiety cues leading as result of poor interoceptive awareness and decreased emotion intensity (Pollatos et al., 2008). Pollatos et al. (2008) conducted a study among females with AN using a heartbeat accuracy test and discovered that they had decreased interoceptive sensitivity compared to healthy control subjects. The authors suggested that this dulled sensitivity to bodily functions is an important factor for onset and perpetuation of psychopathologies. Furthermore, Kerr et al. (2016) discusses that AN patients in their study had abnormal interoceptive activity in their insula which was a contributing factor to subject’s additional anxiety. Interestingly, although AN is known to be associated with a decreased interoceptive awareness of hunger and satiety cues, this study suggests that AN patients may also be hyper-aware of certain gastric sensations in the presence of food due to anticipation of bloating or fullness and subsequently, increasing anxiety (Kerr et al., 2016).

PTSD[edit | edit source]

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is an anxiety disorder characterised by avoidance, hyperarousal, and an impaired ability to change interoceptive states in response to triggering stimuli. Often, people with PTSD have anxiety that stems from anticipation of not being able to properly cope with quickly changing emotional and physical states (Simmons et al., 2009). Research suggests altered activation of the dorso-lateral pre-frontal cortex (DLPFC) and anterior insula in people with PTSD anticipate these interoceptive states and the cognition (anxiety/worry) that comes with this. A disconnection with these areas however has been thought to quicken the physical response to traumatic stimuli and lessen a person’s ability to cope with their response (Simmons et al., 2009). A study conducted by Simmons et al. (2009) discovered subjects had reduced anterior insula and DLPFC activation once being shown aversive stimuli. These subjects therefore lacked both the neural circuitry that prepared the body for physical/emotional/cognitive states, and the ability to distinguish between these bodily states. The study suggested that rather than having decreased interoceptive awareness, subjects more likely had an inability to adapt to interoceptive states.

Study - sexual trauma survivors

Another example of this is seen in female sexual trauma survivors, with researchers suggesting that contrary to their hypothesis, subjects actually had heightened interoceptive accuracy. Additionally, the ability to accurately be aware of interoceptive stimuli (e.g., heartbeat) was consistent with lowered PTSD symptoms among subjects. The study enforces the notion that higher interoceptive accuracy helps to regulate emotions. This is specifically seen in people with PTSD who have lessened emotional symptoms when they are more aware of their body’s internal signals. This research is surprising due to the abundance of research that supports ill mental health and interoceptive dysregulation. It seems that being accurate and aware of oncoming PTSD symptoms means some patients are in fact able to regulate their emotions more effectively than people without PTSD (Reinhardt et al., 2020).

PTSD specifically, can be applied to any of the three theories of emotion mentioned earlier. James-Lange's theory in particular can help to explain how interceptive processes occur after facing aversive/triggering stimuli. The 'interpretation' stage of this hypothesis is the point in which some people become more aware of their heart rate and breathing. It is based on this interpretation that people with PTSD decide what emotion they are experiencing. For people with increased interoceptive accuracy, this means they are aware of their body's reaction and based previously mentioned research, are actually more likely to regulate its emotions. Those who are less able to interpret their body's interoceptive signals however, would be less likely to adapt to changing interoceptive states and the emotions that coincide.

Drug addiction[edit | edit source]

Drug addiction is another devastating mental illness that causes significant emotional, physical and financial issues for people. Drug taking behaviour can be conceptualised as a way to avoid negative physical and mental wellbeing, and so research has found a relationship between this mental illness and interoception via insula dysfunction (Paulus & Stewart, 2014). Drug users are thought to continue using as a way to adjust their optimal level of arousal. By Schachter and Singer's Two-Factor Theory of Emotion, opioid users for example, would consume the drug, experience interoceptive processes such as slowed heart rate and breathing (Victoria Department of Health & Human Services, n.d.) (arousal), become aware of this optimal level of arousal, and then label this emotion as pleasure. A dysfunctional insular cortex would help to explain why drug users continue to seek a certain level of arousal to experience pleasure, and further perpetuate their addiction. With a hypo/hyperactivation of the insula (depending on the drug), comes atypical interoceptive processes that tell the user to continue taking drugs despite negative side effects, as a way to maintain optimal arousal and emotions (Paulus & Stewart, 2014).

Conclusion[edit | edit source]

Interoception is an important process in the human body that helps regulate physiology and emotions in order to maintain homeostasis. Relatively new and exciting research however, has discovered how the ability to accurately be aware of physiological changes in our body is actually related to improved mental health. Theories of emotion help to explain how physiological process may be involved in how emotions come to be, as emotions often coincide with physical reactions. The inability to accurately (or over accurately) be aware of your bodily processes is known as interoceptive dysfunction. As a result of a dysfunctional insular cortex, interoceptive dysfunction can exacerbate or stem from mental health issues due to the its association with emotional dysregulation. Thus, interoceptive and emotional dysregulation can lead to mental illnesses such as MDD, anorexia nervosa, PTSD, and drug addiction.

Because accurately being aware of bodily processes is so clearly linked to mental illness, this newly researched area of psychiatry is extremely important. It is vital for the research to continue to understand how to treat and prevent mental illnesses, in order to improve mental health.

See also[edit | edit source]

References[edit | edit source]

Australia, H. (2019, September 9). Anorexia nervosa. Www.healthdirect.gov.au. https://www.healthdirect.gov.au/anorexia-nervosa

Berntson, G. G., & Khalsa, S. S. (2021). Neural Circuits of Interoception. Trends in Neurosciences, 44(1), 17–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2020.09.011

Brewer, R., Murphy, J., & Bird, G. (2021). Atypical interoception as a common risk factor for psychopathology: A review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 130, 470–508. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.07.036

Cherry, K. (2019, September 30). The Schachter-Singer Two-Factor Theory of Emotion. Verywell Mind; Verywellmind. https://www.verywellmind.com/the-two-factor-theory-of-emotion-2795718

Cherry, K. (2020, September 17). Understanding the Cannon-Bard Theory of Emotion. Verywell Mind; Verywellmind. https://www.verywellmind.com/what-is-the-cannon-bard-theory-2794965

Cherry, K. (2022, October 20). The James-Lange Theory of Emotion. Verywell Mind; Verywellmind. https://www.verywellmind.com/what-is-the-james-lange-theory-of-emotion-2795305

Furman, D. J., Waugh, C. E., Bhattacharjee, K., Thompson, R. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). Interoceptive awareness, positive affect, and decision making in Major Depressive Disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 151(2), 780–785. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.044

Kerr, K. L., Moseman, S. E., Avery, J. A., Bodurka, J., Zucker, N. L., & Simmons, W. K. (2016). Altered Insula Activity during Visceral Interoception in Weight-Restored Patients with Anorexia Nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(2), 521–528. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.174

Khalsa, S. S., Adolphs, R., Cameron, O. G., Critchley, H. D., Davenport, P. W., Feinstein, J. S., Feusner, J. D., Garfinkel, S. N., Lane, R. D., Mehling, W. E., Meuret, A. E., Nemeroff, C. B., Oppenheimer, S., Petzschner, F. H., Pollatos, O., Rhudy, J. L., Schramm, L. P., Simmons, W. K., Stein, M. B., & Stephan, K. E. (2018). Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap. Biological Psychiatry. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(6), 501–513. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004

Khoury, N. M., Lutz, J., & Schuman-Olivier, Z. (2018a). Interoception in Psychiatric Disorders. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 26(5), 250–263. https://doi.org/10.1097/hrp.0000000000000170

Khoury, N. M., Lutz, J., & Schuman-Olivier, Z. (2018b). Interoception in Psychiatric Disorders. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 26(5), 250–263. https://doi.org/10.1097/hrp.0000000000000170

Legg, T. J. (2020, January 28). Alexithymia: Causes, Symptoms, and Treatments. Healthline. https://www.healthline.com/health/autism/alexithymia#diagnosis.

Löffler, A., Foell, J., & Bekrater-Bodmann, R. (2018). Interoception and Its Interaction with Self, Other, and Emotion Processing: Implications for the Understanding of Psychosocial Deficits in Borderline Personality Disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 20(4). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-018-0890-2

Mcleod, S. (2023, October 3). Schachter-Singer Two-Factor Theory of Emotion. Simply Psychology. https://www.simplypsychology.org/schachter-singer-theory.html

Nord, C. L., Lawson, R. P., & Dalgleish, T. (2021). Disrupted Dorsal Mid-Insula Activation During Interoception Across Psychiatric Disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, appi.ajp.2020.2. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20091340

Pasin Neto, H., Bicalho, E., & Bortolazzo, G. (2021). Interoception and Emotion: A Potential Mechanism for Intervention With Manual Treatment. Cureus. https://doi.org/10.7759/cureus.15923

Paulus, M. P., & Stewart, J. L. (2014). Interoception and drug addiction. Neuropharmacology, 76, 342–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.07.002

Pollatos, O., Kurz, A.-L., Albrecht, J., Schreder, T., Kleemann, A. M., Schöpf, V., Kopietz, R., Wiesmann, M., & Schandry, R. (2008). Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 9(4), 381–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.02.001

Prinz, J. J. (2006). Is Emotion a Form of Perception? Canadian Journal of Philosophy Supplementary Volume, 32, 136–160. https://doi.org/10.1353/cjp.2007.0035

Reeve, J. (2018). Understanding Motivation and Emotion (7th ed.). Wiley Global Education US. https://bookshelf.vitalsource.com/books/9781119367659

Reinhardt, K. M., Zerubavel, N., Young, A. S., Gallo, M., Ramakrishnan, N., Henry, A., & Zucker, N. L. (2020). A multi-method assessment of interoception among sexual trauma survivors. Physiology & Behavior, 226, 113108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113108

Simmons, A., Strigo, I. A., Matthews, S. C., Paulus, M. P., & Stein, M. B. (2009). Initial Evidence of a Failure to Activate Right Anterior Insula During Affective Set Shifting in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71(4), 373–377. https://doi.org/10.1097/psy.0b013e3181a56ed8

Torres, F. (2020, October). What is depression? American Psychiatric Association. https://www.psychiatry.org/patients-families/depression/what-is-depression

Victoria Department of Health & Human Services. (n.d.). How drugs affect your body. Www.betterhealth.vic.gov.au. https://www.betterhealth.vic.gov.au/health/healthyliving/How-drugs-affect-your-body#different-drugs-different-effect

Weir, K. (2023, April 1). What is interoception, and how does it affect mental health? Apa.org. https://www.apa.org/monitor/2023/04/sensations-eating-disorders-suicidal-behavior

External Links[edit | edit source]